Witkin and his associates discovered that performance in this task was correlated with performance in other perceptual tasks. For example, those who were strongly influenced by the surrounding frame, the so-called field- dependent observers, also found it difficult to isolate or find familiar figures embedded in a larger pattern. The so- called field-independent observers, who resisted the effect of the frame on the perception of the rod, performed better in the embedded-figure task. The researchers also claimed that the two categories of observers tended to have different kinds of personality. If experimenters use a rectangle sufficiently large to serve as a surrogate frame of reference for the vertical- horizontal coordinates of the environment, and they place it at, say, a tilt of around 30 degrees, observers, on average, will set the rod at roughly 6 or 7 degrees in the direction of the tilted frame. This result implies that a truly vertical rod seen in the frame would appear tilted by 6 or 7 degrees—–a sizable illusion. Quite a few observers do set the rod at this orientation; thus this effect is not simply based on averaging the two extreme types of subject. Such a result has been interpreted as a compromise between the two conflicting determinants: gravity and the visual frame of reference. How is the frame itself perceived? How it is seen presumably would influence the positioning of the rod. Were it to look upright, as does a tilted room in which the observer is enclosed, we should expect that the rod would be set parallel to its edges. Were it to look tilted, as tilted as it really is, should we expect any effect on the perception of the rod at all? If the frame is veridically perceived as tilted by 30 degrees, presumably information about gravity is not being overpowered or captured by the surrogate frame of reference. In that event, the same information ought to be available to the observer to set the rod to the true vertical.